RATING OF HANDLING QUALITIES

To be able to assess aircraft handling qualities one must have a measuring technique with which any given vehicle’s characteristics can be rated. In the early days of avia­tion, this was done by soliciting the comments of pilots after they had flown the air­craft. However, it was soon found that a communications problem existed with pilots using different adjectives to describe the same flight characteristics. These ambigui­ties have been alleviated considerably by the introduction of a uniform set of descrip­tive phrases by workers in the field. The most widely accepted set is referred to as the “Cooper-Harper Scale,’’ where a numerical rating scale is utilized in conjunction with a set of descriptive phrases. This scale is presented in Fig. 1.4. To apply this rating technique it is necessary to describe accurately the conditions under which the results were obtained. In addition it should be realized that the numerical pilot rating (1-10) is merely a shorthand notation for the descriptive phrases and as such no mathemati­cal operations can be carried out on them in a rigorous sense. For example, a vehicle configuration rated as 6 should not be thought to be “twice as bad” as one rated at 3. The comments from evaluation pilots are extremely useful and this information will provide the detailed reasons for the choice of a rating.

Other techniques have been applied to the rating of handling qualities. For exam­ple, attempts have been made to use the overall system performance as a rating pa­rameter. However, due to the pilot’s adaptive capability, quite often he can cause the overall system response of a bad vehicle to approach that of a good vehicle, leading to the same performance but vastly differing pilot ratings. Consequently system per­formance has not proved to be a good rating parameter. A more promising approach involves the measurement of the pilot’s physiological and psychological state. Such methods lead to objective assessments of how the system is influencing the human controller. The measurement of human pilot describing functions is part of this tech­nique (Kleinman et al., 1970; McRuer and Krendel, 1973; Reid, 1969).

Research into aircraft handling qualities is aimed in part at ascertaining which vehicle parameters influence pilot acceptance. It is obvious that the number of possi-

ADEQUACY FOR SELECTED TASK OR
REQUIRED OPERATION*

RATING OF HANDLING QUALITIES

Intense pilot compensation is required to retain control

 

Major deficiencies

 

Control will be lost during some portion of required operation.

 

No

 

Improvement

mandator/

 

Major deficiencies

 

10

 

Pilot

decisions

 

1 Definition of required operation involves designation of flight phase and/or subphase with accompanying conditions.

 

Figure 1.4 Handling qualities rating scale; Cooper/Harper scale (Cooper and Harper, 1969).

 

RATING OF HANDLING QUALITIESRATING OF HANDLING QUALITIES

RATING OF HANDLING QUALITIES

Figure 1.5 Longitudinal short-period oscillation—pilot opinion contours (O’Hara, 1967).

ble combinations of parameters is staggering, and consequently attempts are made to study one particular aspect of the vehicle while maintaining all others in a “satisfac­tory” configuration. Thus the task is formulated in a fashion that is amenable to study. The risk involved in this technique is that important interaction effects can be overlooked. For example, it is found that the degree of difficulty a pilot finds in con­trolling an aircraft’s lateral-directional mode influences his rating of the longitudinal dynamics. Such facts must be taken into account when interpreting test results. An­other possible bias exists in handling qualities results obtained in the past because most of the work has been done in conjunction with fighter aircraft. The findings from such research can often be presented as “isorating” curves such as those shown in Fig. 1.5.